Asking Children to Talk About Abuse: Can Research Help Improve Police Interviewer Skills?

Technological advances in forensic child interview training

Technological advances have also led to new and promising lines of research on forensic child interviewing using the NICHD protocol or similar evidence-based techniques. Researchers at Åbo University in Finland are developing computer software that simulates interviews with young children (Pompedda, Zappalà, & Santtila, 2015). The aim is to provide police interviewers the opportunity to interact to animated child characters (called avatars) in a naturalistic digital environment. The avatars are pre-programmed with background stories that can involve sexual abuse, and the interviewer’s task is to investigate what happened to the avatar. The avatars are programmed with research-based algorithms that correspond to how children typically respond to questions. For example, using invitations and directive questions leads to more correct responses and suggestive questions may elicit false details from the avatars. Experimental studies of this training software have yielded promising results. Trainees have shown improvement in their use of appropriate question types after just one hour of training. In Sweden, researchers have examined a similar digital interview environment where instructors (expert interviewers) have manual control over the animated child characters in the program. (Johansson, 2015). The instructor plays the role of the child in real time and his or her answers are then directly projected into the program through an avatar. Thus, the trainee sees and hears an animated child respond to his or her question in realistic manner. While prototype development is still underway, preliminary results indicate this could be a beneficial approach to forensic child interview training.

In Australia, Benson and Powell (2015) investigated the effects of using interactive web based course modules in forensic child interviewing, where police officers can refresh their knowledge online. In both field interviews and mock interviews with actors, officers adhered more closely to research-based guidelines after the web-based training, compared to before. The concept of easily accessible web-based training seems particularly promising given that regular feedback seems to help police officers stay on track and maintain the new techniques learned in NICHD training (Lamb et al., 2002a). Advanced technology could thus offer an exciting new way forward for practitioners to improve and maintain their forensic child interview skills.

To conclude, the NICHD protocol has helped improve the quality of police interviews with children around the globe. New research continues to investigate techniques for interviewing children who are reluctant to talk. Given that disclosing abuse can be incredibly difficult and daunting for children, this is a hurdle of utmost importance to overcome. At the same time, researchers and practitioners need to keep in mind the flawed nature of human memory, and find ways to encourage disclosures without inadvertently encouraging inaccurate testimony or false memories.

 

References

Ahern, E. C., Hershkowitz, I., Lamb, M. E., Blasbalg, U., & Winstanley, A. (2014). Support and reluctance in the pre-substantive phase of alleged child abuse victim investigative interviews: Revised versus standard NICHD protocol. Behavioural Sciences & the Law, 32, 762-774. doi: 10.1002/bsl.2149  

Benia, L. R., Hauck-Filho, N., Dillenburg, M., & Stein, L. M. (2015). The NICHD investigative interview protocol: A meta- analytic review. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 24, 259-279. doi: 10.1080/10538712.2015.1006749

Benson, M. S., & Powell, M. B. (2015). Evaluation of a comprehensive interactive training system for investigative interviewers of children. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 21, 309-322. doi: 10.1037/law0000052

Brown, D. A., Lamb, M. E., Lewis, C., Pipe, M., Orbach, Y., & Wolfman, M. (2013). The NICHD investigative interview protocol: An analogue study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 19, 367-382. doi:10.1037/a0035143

Bruck, M., & Ceci, S. J. (1999). The suggestibility of children's memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 419-439. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.419

Cederborg, A-C., Alm, C., da Silva Nises, D. L., & Lamb, M. E. (2013). Investigative interviewing of alleged child abuse victims: An evaluation of a new training programme for investigative interviewers. Police Practice and Research, 14, 242-254. doi: 10.1080/15614263.2012.712292.

Cyr, M., Dion, J., McDuff, P., & Trotier-Sylvain, K. (2012). Transfer of skills in the context of non-suggestive investigative interviews: Impact of structured interview protocol and feedback. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26, 516-524. doi: 10.1002/acp.2822

Cyr, M., & Lamb, M. E. (2009). Assessing the effectiveness of the NICHD investigative interview protocol when interviewing French-speaking alleged victims of child sexual abuse in Quebec. Child Abuse and Neglect, 33, 257-268. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2008.04.002

article author(s)

facebook