Children are poor witnesses. Or are they?

Paradoxically, children might be more reliable in situations that are very familiar to adults, but not to children. Especially when there is reference to a unique occurrence and the child makes a spontaneous utterance without previous pressure or influence, their statements should not be left aside. Consequently, in the aforementioned murder case, no external influences provide reason to doubt the 6-year-old girl’s identification of her father as the perpetrator.

References

Bidrose, S., & Goodman, G. S. (2000). Testimony and evidence: A scientific case study of memory for child sexual abuse. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, 197–213. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(200005/06)14:3<197::AID-ACP647>3.0.CO;2-6

Binet, A. (1900). La suggestibilité [The suggestibility]. Paris: Schleicher Freres.

Brackmann, N., Otgaar, H., Sauerland, M., & Jelicic, M. (2014). When children are the least vulnerable to false memories: A true report or a case of autosuggestion? Manuscript submitted for publication.

Brainerd, C. J. (2013). Developmental reversals in false memory: A new look at the reliability of children’s evidence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 335–341. doi:10.1177/0963721413484468

Brainerd, C. J., Reyna, V. F., & Ceci, S. J. (2008). Developmental reversals in false memory: A review of data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 343–382. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.3.343

Brainerd, C. J., Reyna, V. F., & Zember, E. (2011). Theoretical and forensic implications of developmental studies of the DRM illusion. Memory & Cognition, 39, 365–380. doi:10.3758/s13421-010-0043-2

Bruck, M., & Ceci, S. J. (1999). The suggestibility of children’s memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 419–439. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.419

Ceci, S. J., & Bruck, M. (1993). Suggestibility of the child witness: A historical review and synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 403–439. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.113.3.403

Connolly, D. A., & Price, H. L. (2006). Children’s suggestibility for an instance of a repeated event versus a unique event: The effect of degree of association between variable details. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93, 207–223. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2005.06.004

Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 17–22. doi:10.1037/h0046671

Havard, C. (2014). Are children less reliable at making visual identifications than adults? A review. Psychology, Crime & Law, 20(4), 372–388. doi:10.1080/1068316X.2013.793334

Jonker, F., & Jonker-Bakker, I. (1997). Effects of ritual abuse: The results of three surveys in The Netherlands. Child Abuse & Neglect, 21, 541–556. doi:10.1016/S0145-2134(97)00011-2

Loftus, E. F. (2005). Planting misinformation in the human mind: A 30-year investigation of the malleability of memory. Learning & Memory, 12, 361–366. doi:10.1101/lm.94705

Loftus, E. F., Miller, D. G., & Burns, H. J. (1978). Semantic integration of verbal information into a visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 19–31. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.4.1.19

Mason, M. A. (1995). The child sex abuse syndrome: The other major issue in State of New Jersey v. Margaret Kelly Michaels. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 1, 399–410. doi:10.1037/1076-8971.1.2.399

Otgaar, H., Candel, I., Merckelbach, H., & Wade, K. A. (2009). Abducted by a UFO: Prevalence information affects young children’s false memories for an implausible event. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 115–125. doi:10.1002/acp.1445

Otgaar, H., Howe, M. L., Peters, M., Smeets, T., & Moritz, S. (2014). The production of spontaneous false memories across childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 121, 28–41. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2013.11.019

Otgaar, H., Howe, M. L., Smeets, T., Brackmann, N., & Fissette, A. (2014). Changing developmental trends in suggestion-induced false memories. Manuscript submitted for publication.

article author(s)

facebook