Do the Math: Cognitive Load Attenuates Negative Feelings
To conclude, the foregoing work proposes that because cognitive processes and emotion processes compete over limited mental resources, people can take the edge of aversive emotional states by engaging in a cognitive task. Importantly, the degree to which people find relief from their negative feelings may depend on the extent to which people tax their mental resources. Thus, mildly dysphoric moods may be regulated by mildly demanding tasks, such as sorting ones mail, or making a grocery list, whereas intensely negative responses call for more fully engaging activities, such as filling out ones tax form, or taking a course of Mandarin Chinese. So in order to make the Mail Goggles even more effective, in addition to the timing of the application, one should also be able to adjust the difficulty of the math problems: mildly complex ones for an average weekend, and mind-boggling math equations for those gloomy nights right after a romantic break-up.
References
Bantick, S. J., Wise, R. G., Ploghaus, A., Clare, S., Smith, S. M., & Tracey, I. (2002). Imaging how attention modulates pain in humans using functional MRI. Brain, 125, 310-319.
Bargh, J. (1994). The four horsemen of automaticity. In R. S. Wyer (Ed.), Handbook of social cognition (Vol. 9, pp. 1-47). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Bush, G., Whalen, P. J., Rosen, B. R., Jenike, M. A., McInerney, S. C., & Rauch, S. L. (1998). The counting stroop: An interference task specialized for functional neuroimaging - Validation study with functional MRI. Human Brain Mapping, 6, 270-282.
Erber, R. & Tesser, A. (1992). <ahref="http: cat.inist.fr="" ?amodele="afficheN&cpsidt=5387983"">Task effort and the regulation of mood - the absorption hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 28, 339-359.
Erk, S., Kleczar, A., Walter, H. (2007). Valence specific regulation effects in a working memory task with emotional context. Neuroimage, 37, 623-32.
Frankenstein, U. N., Richter, W., McIntyre, M. C., & Remy, F. (2001). Distraction modulates anterior cingulate gyrus activations during the cold pressor test. Neuroimage, 14, 827-836.
Fiedler, K., Nickel, S., Asbeck, J., & Pagel, U. (2003). Mood and the generation effect. Cognition & Emotion, 17, 585–608.
Holmes, E., James, E.J., Coode-Bate, T., & Deeprose, C. (2009). Can playing the computer game "Tetris" reduce the build-up of flashbacks for trauma? A proposal from cognitive science. PLoS ONE, 4, e4153.
Isen, A. M. (2002). Missing in action in the AIM: Positive affect’s facilitation of cognitive flexibility, innovation, and problem solving. Psychological Inquiry, 13, 57–65.
Kavanagh, D. J., Andrade, J., & May, J. (2005). Imaginary relish and exquisite torture: The elaborated intrusion theory of desire. Psychological Review, 112, 446-467.
Kemps, E., Tiggemann, M., & Grigg, M. (2008). Chocolate cravings are susceptible to visuo-spatial interference. Eating Behaviors, 6, 101-107.
Kemps, E., Tiggemann, M., & Hart, G. (2004). Food cravings consume limited cognitive resources. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied, 14, 247-254.
Mogg, K., McNamara, J., Powys, M., Rawlinson, H., Seiffer, A., & Bradley, B. P. (2000). Selective attention to threat: A test of two cognitive models of anxiety. Cognition & Emotion, 14, 375-399.
Morrow, J., & Nolenhoeksema, S. (1990). Effects of responses to depression on the remediation of depressive affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 519-527.